Stereotype threat debunked - People perform better to counteract prejudices toward their group

Posted By News On April 17, 2013 - 3:29pm

In psychology, there are competing speculations about bias. Some claim that if people do poorly on a test, it is because they are already perceived as being poor at it; a small white guy may do poorly at basketball, a woman may do poorly on a math test, because they believe their reputations precede them and that performance pressure ironically causes the under-performance.

On the other side is the idea that individuals from stigmatized groups choose to present themselves in ways that counteract the specific stereotypes and prejudices associated with their group, the focus of a new paper in Psychological Science.

"People often think of prejudice as a simple, single phenomenon — general dislike for members of other groups — but recent research suggests that there are actually multiple, distinct types of prejudice," says graduate student Rebecca Neel, who conducted the research with her advisor Steven Neuberg and post-doctoral scholar Samantha Neufeld at Arizona State University.

Neel and colleagues wanted to see whether people would be aware of the stereotypes associated with their group and whether they would opt for strategies that counteract those specific stereotypes in order to make a good first impression.

The researchers recruited 75 college students, all of whom had self-identified as overweight or not overweight, to participate in a study about "impressions of groups." The students were told that they would answer questions about three groups randomly chosen out of a total pool of ten; in fact, everyone received questions about the same groups: Muslims, Mexican Americans, and obese people.

In a separate part of the study, the students imagined that they were going to meet someone new and ranked eight different strategies for making a good first impression. The strategies included arriving on time, looking interested, smiling, appearing relaxed, and wearing clean clothes.

Some of the participants ranked the eight strategies before receiving the questions about the three groups; others ranked them afterward, so that group-related stereotypes would be fresh in their mind.

Regardless of their own weight, the students perceived conventional stereotypes about obese people. They believed that most people feel disgust toward obese individuals and see them as a threat to their health.

But, just as the researchers predicted, overweight and non-overweight students did show differences in how they ranked the strategies for making a good impression.

Overweight participants who were primed to think about group stereotypes were more likely to prioritize wearing clean clothes than participants in the other conditions — they ranked this strategy, on average, as most important. Non-overweight participants and overweight participants who hadn't been primed tended to give "arriving on time" the highest ranking.

These findings suggest that overweight participants considered wearing clean clothes to be an important strategy for managing other people's first impressions and diminishing the specific emotion — disgust — that underlies prejudice toward obese people.

The results were supported by a second study that included college students from two stigmatized groups: overweight men and Black men.

Once again, the students' reports fell in line with typical stereotypes: Overweight men thought that other people viewed their group as posing a threat of disease, while Black men thought that other people saw their group as posing a threat of violence. And they ranked their impression strategies accordingly.

As before, overweight men ranked wearing clean clothes as more important when stereotypes about obese people were top-of-mind. Black men, on the other hand, viewed smiling — a strategy useful for 'disarming' concerns about ill intentions — as more important when they were primed to think about stereotypes related to African Americans.

Thus, the participants adopted different strategies for managing a first impression, depending on their own group membership and the salience of specific stereotypes and prejudices about their group.

Neel and colleagues argue that this research demonstrates that stigma doesn't manifest as just general negativity; it involves specific emotions that are felt toward specific groups. People's experiences being on the receiving end of these emotions leads them to use different strategies for managing prejudice.

So, whether it's a job interview, a performance evaluation, or a casual social encounter, "members of stigmatized groups may strategically change how they present themselves to others in anticipation of these different emotions," says Neel.

The researchers believe that this research offers a new way of looking at an important social topic:

"Psychology has long been interested in understanding where prejudice comes from, and there's a body of more recent work that seeks to understand prejudice and stereotyping from the target's perspective" the researchers point out. "Our research is part of a growing program that demonstrates the tight links between the psychology of prejudiced perceivers and the psychology of those targeted by these prejudices."

"In psychology, there are competing speculations about bias"

Those views aren't necessarily competing at all. In fact it's more like what is reported here just confirms part of the typical narrative on stereotype threat, except that it finds that the effort to counteract the stereotype sometimes works, rather than backfire.

The performance under stress would vary due to things like more inherent/almost inherent personality traits, and momentary "conditioning" and level of psychological stamina/"ego depletion". Perhaps even more subtle priming effects beforehand, which possibly exist to a degree that make any sort of finding in non-Walden-2 scenarios rather inconveniently noisy. Once I've read about things like week-long effects of looking at a classical versus a modernist painting. And the most famous thing about people supposedly walking slower, more "old-agedly", after being primed by keywords or concepts like "Florida". But again, that's the sort of thing I think is somewhat artificially inflated in study reports, however real the effects may be to a lesser extent.

Not to mention that there are also group and individual genuine differences in ability, regardless of how inherent they are. A shorter guy in basketball is pretty much an example of an inherent difference. Even things like maths (or muscular built), even though not "inherent" in the same way, become "stable" through the lifetime, according to past performance/exercise.

Usually one can't just start to perform consistently at a higher level out of the blue, just with some psychological trick. The effect of that must be quite limited, like the improve in eyesight of pilots under different test circumstances (evoking or failing to evoke a sense of a high stake on the task), but it's not like those weird instances when someone is hit on their head, pass out or enters into a coma, and wakes up being able to speak fluently a foreign language.

If that kind of higher performance could be "hacked" with some psychological trick, that would really be awesome. I wonder if hypnosis can do something that approaches this, even though I' quite skeptical/ignorant of the degree that it's a real thing versus showmanship.

Post new comment

The content of this field is kept private and will not be shown publicly.
  • Allowed HTML tags: <h> <a><em><img><strong><iframe><table><object><cite><p><br><i><b><center><ul><li><div><html5:figure><html5:figcaption><td><tr>
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.

More information about formatting options

CAPTCHA
Sorry, we know you're not a spambot, but they're out there